The Effects of Segregation -- The Pains of Imprisonment
versus Behavioural Deep Freeze
The Arbour recommendation for independent adjudication was not, however,
based solely on the need to ensure compliance with the law and the legal
characterization of segregation. Because, on her assessment of the evidence
and the literature, "There is no rehabilitative effect from long-term
segregation, and every reason to be concerned that it may be harmful,"
placing a prisoner in long-term segregation subjects that prisoner to
greater deprivation than originally envisaged by the sentencing court
and therefore the use, and in her view, the overuse of long-term segregation,
must be subject to independent oversight.
In coming to these conclusions, Madam Justice Arbour reviewed the debate
in the scientific and criminological communities on the effects segregation
has on prisoners who are subjected to it. This was an issue which had
been the subject of evidence in the McCann
case in 1975, and in Prisoners of Isolation
I reviewed the available clinical and empirical research on the subject;
in particular, a series of experiments conducted by Canadian researchers
in the 1960s and 1970s who had concluded that solitary confinement did
not induce any change in a prisoner’s self-identity or blood pressure
and therefore was not demonstrably more stressful than routine prison
life. The problem with these and other experiments was that they bore
no relationship to the typical situation facing prisoners confined in
administrative segregation. The subject of the experiments were volunteers,
the periods of confinement were of short duration (less than 10 days)
and prisoners could terminate their confinement at any point.
In 1990, Dr. Paul Gendreau, the principal researcher in many of the
earlier studies, and a colleague, Dr. James Bonta, published an article
entitled "Re-examining the Cruel and Unusual Punishment of Prison Life"
( Law and Human Behaviour, 1990, vol. 14 at
347). In their article, the authors challenged some of the common assertions
in what they referred to as the "pains of imprisonment" literature, that
imprisonment is inherently detrimental to the humanity of the prisoner,
and argued that based on the "scientific literature," many of the asserted
detrimental effects, including those of solitary confinement, were over-stated
and unsupported. The Bonta and Gendreau review focused on quantitative
studies about the effects of imprisonment and excluded "qualitative or
phenomenological" studies. These latter categories comprised those studies,
like my own, which relied upon personal observation, open-ended interviews
and case studies to draw general conclusions. To be included in the Bonta
and Gendreau analysis, "a study was required to employ objective measures
of the variables of interest and to evaluate the relationship between
them by the means of statistical tests" ( Bonta at 349).
Based only upon such studies, Bonta and Gendreau concluded that while
there was some evidence that prison crowding may produce changes in blood
pressure and self-reported reports of discomfort, "we cannot conclude
that high population density is always associated with aggressive behaviour."
(Bonta at 353) Likewise, the authors found that there was no evidence
that prison had a deleterious effect on the health of prisoners, and in
fact, may "have the fortuitous benefit of isolating the offender from
a highly risky lifestyle in the community." (at 357) Regarding
the effects of long-term incarceration, the authors reviewed various studies
in which scores from psychological tests (for example the MMPI) were compared
among groups of prisoners who had served varying lengths of time in prison
and concluded that "there is little to support the conclusion that long-term
imprisonment necessarily has detrimental effects." (at 359)
On the issue of the effects of solitary confinement, the authors, citing
the experimental studies done in the 1960’s and 70’s (including Dr. Gendreau’s
own work), concluded that these studies "have found few detrimental effects
for subjects placed in solitary confinements for periods up to ten days
. . . Some of the experimental studies even reported beneficial results"
(at 360). In contrast to studies that used volunteer subjects,
the authors cite a 1967 study that looked at twenty prisoners who were
involuntarily placed in solitary confinement for five days which, "using
measures such as cognitive and personality tests, language usage and time
estimation, . . . found no deleterious effects" (Bonta, p. 361). The authors
also referred to a 1982 study that collected data from five prisons in
Canada and the United States and concluded that "in general, inmates found
the first 72 hours the most difficult but after that they adjusted quite
well (at 361). The authors of this
last study further concluded that "our data lend no support to the claim
that solitary confinement . . . is overwhelmingly aversive, stressful
or damaging to the inmates." Bonta and Gendreau were critical of two other
studies conducted in 1966 and 1983 which recorded signs of pathology for
prisoners incarcerated in solitary for periods of up to a year, on the
grounds that "no objective measures or control groups were used." The
1983 study was further criticised because "prisoners were involved in
a class action suit against their keepers and the author actively encouraged
more disclosure when the inmates were not forthcoming with reports of
distress." (at 361)
On the basis of their review of the literature, Bonta and Gendreau concluded:
The real culprit may not necessarily be the condition
of solitary per se but the manner in which the inmates have been treated.
There is evidence suggesting that this is the basis for most inmate complaints
. . . Jackson [in Prisoners of Isolation ]
himself acceded to this fact: When inmates are dealt with capriciously
by management or individual custodial officers, a psychological stress
can be created even in the most humane of prison environments. Therefore,
solitary confinement may not be cruel and unusual punishment under the
humane and time-limited conditions investigated in experimental studies
or in correctional jurisdictions that have well-defined and effectively
administered ethical guidelines for its use. We must emphasise that this
is not an argument for employing solitary and certainly not for the absurdly
lengthy periods as documented by Jackson. (Jackson at 361) Page 1 of 3
|